David Mills, Ph.D., M.A. (physics, psychology)

Note:  An updated and condensed version of this blog is now available as one of the sections of www.MathLearningDifficulties.com

A specific math learning difficulty known as dyscalculia affects about 6% of the population.  This dysfunction is of neurological origin, like dyslexia, but affects a different brain center and has its own cluster of symptoms.   While it affects the same number of people as does dyslexia, unfortunately it is much less well known,  so dyscalculia is frequently missed and not treated effectively.

A young student tries very hard to master a concept in math, finally seems to understand it, but the next day or week cannot remember how to do it and has to start over. This occurs over and over.

A child has extreme difficulty learning and retrieving math "facts," such as addition and multiplication tables, or in applying techniques such as subtraction and division, or in understanding fractions.

An adult colleague, a specialist in learning disorders, reports:
I truly don't have a concept of numbers.  I got through school with Bs in in algebra and trig by memorizing formulas and plugging in numbers to get answers that meant nothing to me.  I can understand the mechanics of addition, subtraction, multiplication and division.  Fractions take more effort... My ACT scores were all above 30 except I got 18 in math.

A high school physics student I was tutoring got an answer of 1 million meters for the wavelength of a sound wave, and had no idea that this was at all incorrect (The correct answer was 1 meter).

This math learning difficulty is due to a dysfunction of genetic/neurological origin called developmental dyscalculia, or usually just dsycalculia.  Math learning difficulties or disabilities that are acquired by accident, stroke or other illness are called, in distinction, acalculia. In practice, a student with dyscalculia is defined as one who has a very difficult time learning mathematics appropriate to their age, in the absence of other causative factors such as accident, poor previous instruction, poor home environment, low IQ, or emotional illness (including math anxiety).  Most often, students who are probably dyscalculic are identified by choosing among the students who fall in the lowest one-sixth of students given a standard math achievement test, but who have IQs above 80.

Dyscalculia that shows up early will be the main focus of this section, that is, dyscalculia that is diagnosable by second or third grade.  At present, it is still unclear if there are forms of dyscalculia which do not cause early trouble with learning mathematics but which do cause trouble later.  Further, most studies of dyscalculia have focused on children in second grade and older.  A diagnosis of dyscalculia cannot reliably be made much earlier, because some symptoms of dyscalculia are normal responses at earlier ages.

SIGNS OF EARLY-ONSET DYSCALCULIA

Poor subitizing.  The earliest sign that dyscalculic students have a more difficult time than their peers is in the first number task: subitizing and the associated visual counting.  Subitizing is the ability to know how many of a small number of objects there are without counting them, e.g., from a brief glance. Human babies at birth have the ability to subitize, and other primates have very similiar (homologous) neural circuits:  Obviously, subitizing evolved early in primate evolution (or before) because it is a survival trait.  In humans, the subitizing function is located in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) illustrated in the diagram below.  Subitizing is an innate ability that does not have to be learned -- although it can be improved with training  (Fischer, et al., 2008;  Groffman, 2009).   Children with poor subitizing appear to have difficulty with most subsequent aspects of math, detailed as follows.

Counting to ten.  The next task is that of learning to count.  Children naturally learn to count on their fingers, and in fact, the brain center that is involved in counting is located in or near the brain center that provides motor control of the fingers (see Souza, 2008).  This counting module is in a completely different part of the brain from the subitizing module.  Dyscalculic children have a much more difficult time than their peers in understanding the process of counting and of learning "number facts" such as 4 + 3 = 7, or understanding why it is the same answer as 3 + 4. 

Constructing a mental number line is the next task. This refers to the ability to order the numbers along a line from one to ten, to recognize that 9 is bigger than 7, etc.  Here again, dyscalculic children have more difficulty with this task than age-matched controls, taking longer to compare the relative locations of two numbers, for example. 

Learning number facts.  In general, dyscalculic individuals have a much more difficult time learning and using "number facts," such as addition and subtraction, multiplication tables, etc.  It is not clear whether the problem lies in the transfer to long-term memory itself, or in the ability to retrieve the appropriate item from long-term memory, i.e., using the "associative" memory function.

Educators who work with both dyslexics and dyscalculics (e.g., Cathy Davis, Director,  Excelsior Academy) say that the easiest way to diagnose the existence of either condition is that the student can learn something one day, really seem to understand it well, but the next day they have forgotten it completely or at least cannot retrieve it from memory  -- and this occurs in the area of their difficulty only.  Repetition by itself does not seem sufficient.  The dyscalculic has to relearn the same "fact" again and again, and may never be able to "learn" (access from long-term memory) some math facts.  The frustration (and worse) that this causes an intelligent learner is well described in a "Letter to my Math Teacher" on the web site www.dyscalculia.org.

Finally, estimating and counting appear to involve two different functions, the estimating ability apparently arising naturally as an extension of the subitizing function, while counting arises in the finger motor control area.  Dyscalculics are notably poor at estimating, e.g., knowing when a math answer makes sense, etc. 

ORIGIN IN BRAIN "NUMBER MODULE"
 
There is evidence that the primary neural deficit that causes dyscalculia lies in a specific number module (Butterworth, 2005).  Adding the evidence contributed by Fischer et al. (2008) leads to the conclusion that the specific number module affected must involve the subitizing function.  The way that a basic dysfunction in subitizing (and visual counting) could lead to the other symptoms of dyscalculia is as follows.  Learning to understand, count and add small numbers efficiently requires the use of an effective subitizing (visualizing) function (see Benoit, et al., 2004).  This would explain why dyscalculics have such a difficult time with this task.

The next step, as Sousa (2008) suggests, is learning the mental number line, seeing numbers from small to large arranged along a line.  This requires a combination of the subitizing and counting functions, which is potentially problematic even for normal learners because the two centers are separated by a large distance in the brain.  For dyscalculics, who have difficulty with both counting and subitizing, it would seem to be doubly difficult to obtain a good sense of the mental number line.

A deficit in the subitizing module therefore explains well the main difficulties of the dyscalculic student at the early stages.  In addition, Butterworth, et al. (2011) note that, with typical learners, the number module is routinely accessed even during the routine task of retrieving simple arithmetic facts from memory.  Therefore, because the function of the number module is disturbed in dyscalculics, they have (much) more difficulty learning and remembering simple arithmetic facts than do typical learners.

In support of the subitizing-deficit conclusion, we note that  about 6% of all students have dyscalculia according to present estimates.  This is very nearly the same as estimates for the fraction of students with dyslexia, defined similarly to dyscalculia but with regard to reading and other language difficulties.  These two groups overlap:  about 60% of students with either condition have both.  It is also true that dyslexic students, because of trouble with language, will have some characteristic difficulties with learning mathematics.

However, it has recently been convincingly shown that the symptoms caused by (at least early) dyscalculia and those caused by dyslexia do not overlap. The two conditions appear to not have the same neurological cause, and they are additive in their effects (Landerl, et al., 2009).  This finding supports the conclusion that dyscalculia involves a dysfunction in a specific number module.  Combined with the findings by Fischer, et al., we come to the tentative conclusion that the dysfunction at least must involve the subitizing part of the number module.

The situation as it now stands is illustrated with the diagram to the left, which shows that only three genes or genetic factors (A, B, C) are necessary to explain the observations (although of course the actual genetic cause may be much more complicated).  To make the numbers simple, we assume that 8% of the population have dyscalculia, and the same fraction have dyslexia.  The total affected is not 16%, however, because of overlap.  We assume, consistent with the data, that 5 of every 8 people (62.5%) with either condition have both conditions (middle group at bottom of diagram).

The diagram suggests that the conclusions of many of the earlier studies have to be regarded with care because many of these studies did not test independently for reading problems and so they were actually studying a mixed group, comprised of the two groups on the left.   It is not surprising that their conclusions were consequently complex.  In contrast, Landerl, et. al. (2009) did divide their subject population into the three groups, and found the results much easier to understand.   It is also interesting that the proportions of subjects in the three groups in their study were in the ratios suggested by the figure -- even though their selection criteria was simply based on math and reading scores.

The crucial importance of good subitizing in normal development was shown in a longitudinal study in a random group of first graders.  Penner-Wilger, et al., 2007) measured three components suspected of importance in the subsequent development of arithmetic skills:  Subitizing ability, finger sensitivity (gnosia) and finger motor control (finger tapping facility).  They then measured the subsequent development of number system knowledge (understanding numbers, counting, cardinality, etc.) and calculation skill (knowing and using math facts accurately).   The diagram on the left shows their main conclusions, with the width of the arrow lines illustrating the main connections.

Good subitizing ability contributed independently to number system knowledge AND calculation skill (see the two arrows).  In contrast, the systems associated with learning counting (finger skills)  contributed directly only to number system knowledge, which subsequently had a strong effect on calculation skill, as would be expected.   Even though subitizing has no obvious connection with learning math facts, therefore, this research illustrates that there is an important connection independent of subitzing's direct effect on basic number system understanding.

Finally, a recent NIH report confirms these findings, summarizing a study by Mazzocco, et al. (2011).  They report that a deficit in the subitizing/estimating module was very common in students who consistently tested in the lowest  10% in math achievement.  However, there was no such correlation for  those students in the next lowest group, those testing in the 11 to 25% range.  This fits in with our view that there are several different reasons for math learning difficulties, that a deficit in the subitizing/estimating module underlies those having the most difficulty, and this comprising about 6% of the population.

BRAIN TRAINING CAN IMPROVE MATH PERFORMANCE

In their review, Butterworth et al. (2011) note that the typical educational approach to helping  dyscalculics focuses on remedial work to fill in their conceptual and operational gaps, e.g.,  using various techniques to help them learn and retrieve arithmetic facts.  These techniques usually include lots of repetition, which is stressful for the student and time consuming of teacher and special educator's time.  They suggest, "In contrast, neuroscience research suggests that rather than address isolated conceptual gaps, remediation should build the foundational number concepts first."  They go on to review several software programs designed to do this.  However, they do not, for some reason, include the most obvious, the direct training of subitizing as discussed next.

The possibility that subitzing practice can help dyscalculics is derived from other results reported in Fischer's papers.  They demonstrated that even a small amount of training in subitizing improved the ability of the  (probably dyscalculic) children to subitize, and showed this subitizing improvement resulted in a significant increase in mathematics ability!

The papers report that students were given small portable instruments to take home to practice subitizing on.  The software started with identification of small numbers of objects, consistent with the students' initial abilities, but then increasing numbers were shown as ability increased.   Students were trained on the machine for three weeks at 15 minutes per day.  Software in the machine allowed the study directors to verify the amount of training, to monitor the individual student's improvement and to adjust the software appropriately.

After only three weeks of training, the dyscalculic students subitizing ability improved sufficiently that they now tested, on average, in the normal range.  More important, their scores on a standard mathematics test showed a small (but significant) improvement after even this short a time.  Most important, following the training, the dyscalculic student math scores continued to improve in the classroom for the following year with only normal mathematics instruction.  A year later, Fischer reports (personal communication) that the math skills of students who had been treated with the subitizing practice (for only three weeks) had continued to improve compared to students who had not gotten the training.  This included learning math facts!

In other words, the subitizing training allowed the students to improve in areas of mathematics which are not at all obviously related to the ability to subitize.  This seems to me to be convincing proof that the core deficit common to (at least) early-onset dyscalculia is a defect in the subitizing module.

If you have a young child who is showing signs of dyscalculia, I think it would certainly be reasonable to make the minimal investment required for several weeks practice with subitizing.   Further information on access to available programs is discussed below.

It seems possible that older students and adults with dyscalculia might be helped with such a program as well.


PROGRAMS FOR SUBITIZING IMPROVEMENT 

●  In the United States, S. Groffman has developed a subitizing practice program, SUB iNet, produced by Home Therapy Systems.  It runs on both Windows and Mac.

This program is straightforward and well-designed.  The student is taken through a series of symbols flashed on a screen for short time, and told if their answer is correct.  The program starts with one or two symbols and increases slowly in difficulty as the student's improvement allows.  A young child would have to be instructed on how to use the program  initially but should be able to proceed on their own after that.  It does not have a childish game to contend with, so it would be suitable for people of any age to use.  However, this can be a drawback with young children, as they may need adult encouragement to persist in practicing for the 15 minutes a day required.  However, of those available, this program would be my first recommendation for serious work on the improvement of subitizing for a person of any age with dyscalculia.   This program is available through optometrists and other qualified professionals.

It seems to me that there is a significant problem with the screening test included in this program.  This screening test flashes  a number of objects on the screen for 0.2 seconds and the student then reports the number seen.  Each number of objects (one to six) is shown twice in random order, so a perfect score equals 12 correct responses.  While the test does keep track of the average time for the response, it does not use this information effectively.  Rather, the pass/fail criterion is only based on the total number correctly identified out of 12 presentations.  This is a significant problem because, as Fischer et al. (2008) clearly show, their test subjects (poor math students) have two problems with their subitizing/visual counting function: accuracy AND speed of response.  For example, for the case with five items presented, on average the test subjects got the number of items correct 80% of the time, while the controls did only slightly better at 90% correct.    The average response time of the controls was 2.0 seconds for this same number of objects, compared to 2.6 seconds for the test subjects  (these reactions times include only correct responses). 

My reasons for this concern come from recent experience field-testing this program:  I have used the pass/fail screen on some students who are clearly early-onset dyscalculic (based on other symptoms and history) but who appear to be able to use a good visual memory to make up for their (probable) deficiency in subitizing.  (This is exactly what one student spontaneously reported doing!)  That is, some students with probable dyscalculia could score a perfect 12 on the screening test but take longer than average to respond due to having to visually count all of the objects.  In contrast, a person with good subitizing ability does not have to visually count the items in their subitizing range, so a normal person will respond faster on average.  However, the average response time for normal children was not reported for this screening test so only the total correct is made available for evaluation. 

My tentative conclusion is that this program is still the best available for practicing subitizing but that the screening test it contains should not be used alone to determine who is a potential subject for the training:  The screening test results are useful but the information is incomplete, a decision on using the training program must be supplemented by a careful history of symptoms (as described above).

●  An adaptive game for dyscalculic children has recently been developed:  Number Bonds is now available at www.number-sense.co.uk/numberbonds/It is played online.  It is obviously still in development, as it does not really tell the student what to do.  It is, however, easy enough to figure out.

The student is presented with an opening screen like the one on the left.  For this particular task, the screen has only even sizes of bars from zero to 10 units long. In the left column, a bar of length 2 to 10 units starts floating down.  The student's task is to click on one of the bars on the right which will add up to a length of ten units.  When the student does so, the other bar then appears and both bars continue down, adding to the stack of correctly completed bars shown.  (The correct choice in this case is the zero at the top.)  If the student does not click on the correct bar,  both bars disappear when they reach the current stack, and a new bar appears at the top.  The game is adaptive:  If the student takes longer to answer, the bar falls more slowly.  Only when the student has completed the task for all combinations of the bars does the program say "well done" and move to the next screen.

The sequence of screens includes colored even rods (shown), colored odd rods, then colored both; followed by  similar sequences with uncolored rectangles and then rectangles labeled with a number giving their correct length, both colored and blank; and finally numbers appear without bars.  The object of the program is to teach number sense, the connection between the number and the length it represents, and to drive home the concept of commutivity.  As illustrated, the top two bars in the stack demonstrate physically how 8+2 = 2+8.  Also, the program obviously allows repeated concrete practice of the math facts for the number pairs adding to ten.

The Number Race is a free open-source software program available online.  It is currently available to run on Windows 2000, XP, and Vista.  It is available in a number of languages and has a variety of modules intended to teach early number concepts.  It is intended for children aged 4-8 years, and engages the child by asking him or her to play a simple game.  This is probably useful for children of that age range but would potentially get in the way of practice by older students or adults.  Also, this program does not specifically include subitizing practice, but does include lots of practice in comparing sizes of numbers, etc.

In Europe, B. Fischer has developed a network of partner institutions who offer diagnosis, followed by training using stand-alone portable devices to be used at home.   The diagnostic procedure used not only checks subitizing ability but eye movement abilities required to read and do math.


ADDITIONAL REFERENCES AND RESOURCES

 B. Butterworth, S. Varma, D. Laurillard, "Dyscalculia:  From brain to education," Science, 232, 1049-1053 (May 27, 2011).  An excellent recent review article, includes a review of computer programs for helping dyscalculic children.  They recommend, in general, that such programs be used developed and used extensively in educating these children in place of more costly methods, e.g., special needs teachers using physical manipulatives such as  Cuisenaire rods, etc.

L. Benoit, H. Lehalle, F. Jouen, "Do Young Children Acquire Number Words Through Subitizing or Counting?" Cognitive Develop., 19 (2004).   They conclude that subitizing is required "for acquiring the meaning of the first few number words."

B. Butterworth, "Developmental Dyscalculia," in Handbook of Mathematical Cognition, J. Campbell (Ed) 455-467 (2005).

J. Emerson, P. Babtie, The Dyscalculia Assessment (2010) .  An instruction manual for a detailed assessment of dyscalculia and other math difficulties including suggestions for interpreting and using the assessment to plan the intervention strategy.  Primarily for grade school children of normal intelligence having particular difficulty with math, intended for teachers and other special needs professionals.

B. Fischer et al., "Subitizing and Visual Counting in Children with Problems in Acquiring Basic Arithmetic Skills," Optometry & Visual Develop., 39, 24-29 (2008).

B. Fischer et al., "Effects of Daily Practice on Subitizing, Visual Counting, and Basic Arithmetic Skills," Optometry & Visual Develop., 39, 30-34 (2008).

S. Groffman, "Subitizing:  Vision Therapy for Math Deficits," Optometry & Vision Develop., 40, 229-238 (2009).

K. Landerl et al., "Dyslexia and dyscalculia: Two learning disorders with different cognitive profiles," J. Exper. Child Psychol., 103, 309-324 (2009). 

N. Penner-Wilge, L. Fast, J. LeGevre, B. Smith-Chant, S. Skwarchuk, D. Kamawar, J. Bisan, "The foundations of numeracy:  Subitizing, finger gnosia, and fine motor ability," conference proceedings archive, PDF (2007).

D. Sousa, How the Brain Learns Mathematics (2008).

Dyscalculia.org is a very useful web site devoted to math learning disabilities, with moving personal reports from those suffering from the condition.

The website for the National Center for Learning Disabilities is an excellent resource for all learning difficulties. 

The Learning Disabilities Association of America conducts an annual conference open to public and professionals that contains (a small proportion of) sessions devoted to dyscalculia.

All-Ways Learning, by Dr. Dawn Romano, offers full service assistance to those with learning disabilities in the New Orleans area.


ADDITIONAL INFORMATION ON IMPORTANT RESEARCH REPORTS and COMMENTS
 ADDITIONAL INFORMATION ON IMPORTANT RESEARCH REPORTS

K. Landerl, B. Fussenegger, K. Moll, E. Willburger, "Dyslexia and dyscalculia:  Two learning disorders with different cognitive profiles," J. Exper. Child Psychology, 103, 309-324 (2009). 

This study was of Austrian children in grades 2-4, with I.Q.s above 85, and no diagnosed ADHD.  Children were divided into four groups as follows:  a) dyscalculic only: math scores in lowest 16% (lowest 1/6) for age, normal reading scores (20 students); b) dyslexic only: reading scores in lowest 1/6, normal math scores (21 students); c) dyscalculic/dyslexic: both reading and math scores in lowest 1/6 (mixed or "co-morbid" group, 26 students), and d) normal/control children with reading and math scores in the normal range for their age (42 students).  This study found that both dyslexic groups, irrespective of arithmetic defects, had a phonological deficit.  Perhaps surprisingly,  in one test -- reading random digits -- the dyslexic children had significant defects, whereas the dyscalculic only (and normal children) did not.  Children with dyscalculia, irrespective of reading deficits, had difficulties with number sense, e.g., in processing of symbolic and non-symbolic magnitudes. As the abstract summarizes, "Cognitive defects in the comorbid dyslexia/dyscalculia group were additive; that is, they resulted from the combination of two learning disorders  These findings suggest that dyslexia and dyscalculia have separable cognitive profiles, namely a phonological defect in the case of dyslexia and a deficient number module in the case of dyscalculia."

Note that the numbers of children in each group were suggestive of what has been estimated overall:  As noted above, there were about 20 children in the dyscalculic only and dyslexic only groups, and 26 children that had  both.  This is in agreement with published estimates that suggest that any child diagnosed with either dyscalculia or dyslexia has about a 60% chance of having both.  This means that any previous study which purported to study dyslexia, but which did not exclude dyscalculia, contained a population in which the majority of subjects actually had dyscalculia as well.  This implies that the reports of math difficulties resulting from dyslexia in such studies are suspect, because most of the participants actually had dyscalculia as well.  The same, of course, goes for any study of dyscalculia which did not exclude dyslexia. 

Overall, the study suggests that people with dyscalculia have a specific deficit in a "number module" which is involved with number sense, specifically in understanding magnitudes.  Children with dyslexia have a defect in a "phonological module," and the difficulties they have with mathematics are due to difficulties in reading and in understanding what they read, NOT in the number sense itself.  The frequent co-occurrence of both dyslexia and dyscalculia, of course, suggests there is an underlying common genetic/developmental predisposition, even though this study strongly suggests that completely different brain centers are affected.

Finally, note that this research cannot indicate what specific math difficulties might be caused by dyslexia alone, for the simple reason that students with dyslexia alone were identified as students with poor reading skills but with math skills in the normal range.  One would need to establish both dyslexia and dyscalculia clinically with a battery of tests, not just reading or math scores, to be able to investigate this issue correctly.  For similar reasons, interpretation of the study for the diagnosis of dyscalculia is limited, because the range of scores on the tests is characteristic of the whole group of the poorest 1/6 in math.  (See comments on a note by Fischer below)


Patrick Schleifer and Karin Landerl, "Subitizing and counting in typical and atypical development," Developmental Sci., 14, 280-291 (2011)

Abstract:
Enumeration performance in standard dot counting paradigms was investigated for different age groups with typical and atypically poor development of arithmetic skills. Experiment 1 showed a high correspondence between response times and saccadic frequencies for four age groups with typical development. Age differences were more marked for the counting than the subitizing range. In Experiment 2 we found a discontinuity between subitizing and counting for dyscalculic children; however, their subitizing slopes were steeper than those of typically developing control groups, indicating a dysfunctional subitizing mechanism. Across both experiments a number of factors could be identified that affect enumeration in the subitizing and the counting range differentially. These differential patterns further support the assumption of two qualitatively different enumeration processes.
 Comments: This research adds much needed data on subitizing and counting to the above research.  Here, they define dyscalculic children as those 1.5 standard deviations below mean on standardized arithmetic tests (i.e., in the lowest 6.7%) so that the group is likely to have a much higher fraction of actual dyscalculics than the group defined above (the lowest 16%).  Again, they excluded normal readers from the dyscalculic sample, so this paper has only  "dyscalculia only" and normal groups.  For the subitizing range (1-3 items) they find for the dyscalculic that the response times (RTs) are larger and the slope (increase in RT with number of objects) is also larger.  They conclude, however, that this difference is significant, whereas they state in the discussion that there is not an important difference in the slope in the counting range (4-9) between dyscalculic and normal.  However, Fig. 2 clearly shows that the dyscalculic (dotted lines) RTs are about 1/2 second higher than normals, especially for grades 3 and 4 --  i.e., dyscalculic children and normals take significantly longer than normals in getting accurate answers in both the subitizing and counting ranges, and the difference in time between the groups increases steadily with the number of objects.  (It also increases with the age of the child between 2nd and 3rd grade because the normal children RTs improve more than the dyscalculic children across the range -- a hallmark of a developmental problem.)



The first study was of children age 7 to 17 years, identified by having poor math scores but normal reading scores, I.Q. > 85 and no diagnosed ADHD.  However, there were only 9 dyscalculic ("test") children in the older age range (14-17) so that results for test subjects in this age range are best ignored.  In all cases, the stimuli were 1-9 dots in a random grid presented very briefly.  Children with poor math performance incorrectly identified the number of dots and took longer to answer even when correct than did age-matched control subjects.  The difference increased with increasing age.  In the 11-13 age group, for effective recognition (per cent correct divided by response time), nearly 80% of the test group (4 of 5) fell at or below the scores of the lowest 16% (1/6) of the control group.  This is a very significant and impressive difference compared to typical results in studies involving complex behavior!

In the second study, children from 7 to 13 years previously identified as having poor subitizing skill (those in the lowest 1/6 of all children their age) were given practice in subitizing at home using a small hand-held device.  Only 15 minutes a day of practice was required for three weeks.  The number of dots presented started in the low range and was increased only as the children's skill increased.  At the end of the three weeks, 85% of the subjects had improved on their subitizing ability so that their scores fell within the normal range.  More important, their average arithmetic scores, measured by a standard test, had also improved significantly at the end of the three weeks time.  The change in scores was not large in absolute terms at the end of three weeks.  However, Dr. Fischer (personal communication) reports that the arithmetic scores for the trained children continued to improve over the year following their training, the children having had only normal classroom instruction in arithmetic and no additional subitizing training during that time. 

These two papers by Fischer and colleagues, taken together with the Landerl, et al. (2009) results, suggest strongly that there is a defect in the "number module" in dyscalculia and that this defect specifically involves the ability to subitize.  Subitizing is an innate ability, pre-wired in the brain, and does not have to be learned.  Human babies can apparently do it from birth and homologous circuits are present in all primates. The ability is obviously a survival trait that has evolved for good reason. However innate, the importance of the research of Fischer, et al., is that subitizing ability can be improved with training, and that such improvement leads to an improvement in arithmetic skill.

Note the disclaimer by Fisher in the first paper that his results do NOT necessarily prove anything specific about dyscalculia per se.  The reason is that their selection criteria included only that the student score in the bottom 1/6 (16%) in math tests and have an IQ > 85.  There was no independent selection for dyscalculia, and indeed there is not yet a simple test that can reliably do so (in any single test so far there is significant overlap between normal children and probable-dyscalculic children).  If one assumes that 6% of all children have dyscalculia, AND that they will fall in the lower 16% of the class (probably reasonable), then dyscalculic children would make up only 1/3 of his test children.  To have seen a significant result in the whole group, if the result applies primarily to dyscalculic children, makes the study even more impressive.  However, it is just our working hypotheses -- but one supported by other studies -- that dyscalculic children in general suffer from poor subitizing, and going even further, that they will benefit from subitizing practice.

 
N. Penner-Wilge, L. Fast, J. LeGevre, B. Smith-Chant, S. Skwarchuk, D. Kamawar, J. Bisan, "The foundations of numeracy:  Subitizing, finger gnosia, and fine motor ability," conference proceedings archive, PDF (2007).

Shows that of the three "foundations," subitizing ability has the highest correlation to number system knowledge and in addition subitizing contributes directly to the subsequent calculation skill, where the other foundational skills do not.  This study of an unselected population supports the hypothesis that poor subitizing ability by itself could be the root cause of early-onset dyscalculia, including difficulty in acquiring calculation skills.

Note:  All original material on this and linked websites © D. Mills 2011.

6 comments:

  1. Hello Dr Mills,

    An excellent article.

    My experience developing math approaches over the last 27 years had led me to the conclusion that the horizontal number line should be banned.

    Apart from the fact West being negative and East being positive makes little sense in a modular sense, most if not all children perceive higher and lower numbers as they relate to the real world in a vertical plane.

    Has anybody else researched whether or not horizontal number lines are associated with directional confusion?

    Thank you,
    Jonathan Crabtree
    Project Director
    Australian Numerals
    P.S. I am a naïve mathematician.

    ReplyDelete
  2. Thank you for the prompt response David.

    Perhaps if you have some time to kill, you might enjoy a YouTube clip?

    At 27 seconds, select the rocky boat option for the number line video.
    http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=1GppjWY1OqI

    ReplyDelete
  3. Jonathan,
    What you say makes a lot of sense. Thermometers rise with temperature, higher energy things go higher, people who are taller are higher, why shouldn't the number line go from down to up?
    Comments from Renee Newman on dyscalculia.org suggest to me that dyscalculics do experience directional confusions, with, e.g., with fractions. Why make understanding the number line any more confusing than it is?
    To answer your question, I have not yet read about this issue in published work, but I will keep an eye out for it.

    ReplyDelete
  4. Many thanks, David, for the concise yet detailed account of these areas of intervention re. dyscalculia.

    I admire and welcome the clarity and succinct nature of your descriptions and explanations.

    chris - primary teacher trainee New Zealand

    ReplyDelete
  5. I have been doing the sub program. Not sure if I'm getting any better at math but I am better at recognizing the number of objects up to 5 or sometimes 6. 4 has become a piece of a cake whereas it wasn't before. I tried using the program with one of my students. He's a 4h grader with asperger's. Unfortunately the exercises didn't hold his attention or provide the kind of immediate feedback kids get with video games. I'm not sure most kids would work through the program. That's a shame. It does get monotonous, but I want to improve in math so I persevere. My recommendation would be to provide more positive feedback between levels and to divide the levels into more so that there is a chance of accomplishment sooner. I'll keep you posted.

    ReplyDelete
  6. Hello, I'm a college student who lives in Los Angeles and I think I may be suffering from dyscalculia because I have taken the same math class 3 times and I have failed. Can anyone tell me where I can go to get tested to see if I actually have it.

    Please help,
    Ebony
    honeegrrl@gmail.com

    ReplyDelete